Light on human social structure

Biological continuity from chimpanzees to australopithecines.

June 02, 2011 10:57 pm | Updated 10:57 pm IST

"Mrs. Ples," the most famous example of Australopithecus africanus from Sterkfontein cave in South Africa in this undated photo.

"Mrs. Ples," the most famous example of Australopithecus africanus from Sterkfontein cave in South Africa in this undated photo.

Researchers studying the diet of human ancestors who lived two million years ago in southern Africa have unexpectedly come across a crucial clue to their social structure: The males never strayed far from home, and the females dispersed after puberty to neighbouring groups.

The pattern of female dispersal is not unexpected, since it is practised by chimpanzees, the closest living species to humans, and by some hunter-gatherers. But social behaviour does not fossilise, and any data on the subject is invaluable in reconstructing how human social structure evolved.

“This is the first direct evidence that we have about the residence pattern of hominins,” said Bernard Chapais, a primatologist and expert on human social evolution at the University of Montreal.

The evidence emerged from study of the fossil teeth of 19 australopithecines , the still apelike ancestors of the human lineage. The makeup of teeth can reflect local geography because some chemicals like strontium are drawn from rocks into plants, and then into tooth enamel when the plants are eaten. In australopithecines , the absorption of strontium continued until the teeth were completed around age eight.

The researchers were measuring different versions, or isotopes, of strontium to see if the australopithecines travelled far from home in search of food. As the data rolled out of the measuring device, “we were at first disappointed”, said Julia Lee-Thorp of Oxford University, a member of the research team, “but we soon realised that we had found another prize”.

This was that half the females had been born far away, whereas all the males had grown up locally, the team, led by Sandi R. Copeland of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Germany, reported Wednesday in the journal Nature .

In most mammals, the females stay in the home community and the males disperse after adolescence to avoid inbreeding. But chimpanzees and many human hunter-gatherer groups are unusual in following the opposite pattern. The reason may have to do with the aggressive territoriality of both species: A group of males who have grown up with one another is more cohesive and better at defending a territory against competitors. This obliges the females to be the gender that disperses.

“It's really nice to see there is biological continuity from chimpanzees to australopithecines ,” said Joan B. Silk, an expert on primate social behaviour at the University of California, Los Angeles.

The joint ancestor of chimps and humans lived some five million years ago and is often assumed to have had a chimplike social structure, with a male hierarchy, promiscuous mating by the females and all-out war between neighbouring bands. The central puzzle of human social evolution, in Mr. Chapais' view, is to explain how promiscuity was replaced by pair bond and aggressive relations were pushed up from the band level to that of the tribe, a group of bands tied together by exchange of women.

The australopithecines studied by Mr. Copeland's team still had a somewhat chimplike social structure, said Mr. Chapais, because the pair bond did not evolve until the appearance of Homo erectus around 1.8 million years ago. The australopithecine fossils came from the Sterkfontein valley, and one of them has been dated to 2.2 million years ago.

The pair bond, in his view, arose when human ancestors started to develop more dispersed sources of food and males increasingly guarded a group of females to protect both them and their own paternity. With the male around anyway, he could also help rear the children, which allowed for a longer period of juvenile dependence and hence for brain size to grow. The relatively small size of the australopithecine brain suggests that this process had not yet started, said Mr. Chapais. Another far-reaching consequence of the developing pair bond was that individuals could at last start to recognise their relatives, which chimpanzees mostly cannot do. So when females dispersed to neighbouring groups, males from their home community could recognise their daughters or sisters, together with their in-laws, who had an equal interest in their children's welfare. This transformed the neighbouring group from an adversary to an ally, and the human social structure expanded to being that of a tribe. Friendly relations, in Mr. Chapais' view, then allowed males as well as females to transfer, the pattern typical of hunter-gatherers today. — New York Times News Service

0 / 0
Sign in to unlock member-only benefits!
  • Access 10 free stories every month
  • Save stories to read later
  • Access to comment on every story
  • Sign-up/manage your newsletter subscriptions with a single click
  • Get notified by email for early access to discounts & offers on our products
Sign in

Comments

Comments have to be in English, and in full sentences. They cannot be abusive or personal. Please abide by our community guidelines for posting your comments.

We have migrated to a new commenting platform. If you are already a registered user of The Hindu and logged in, you may continue to engage with our articles. If you do not have an account please register and login to post comments. Users can access their older comments by logging into their accounts on Vuukle.